Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Biliteracy and acquisition of novel written words: the impact of phonological conflict between L1 and L2 scripts

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The acquisition of new orthographic representations is a rapid and accurate process in proficient monolingual readers. The present study used biliterate and bialphabetic population to address the impact of phonological inconsistencies across the native (L1) and second (L2) alphabets. Naming latencies were collected from 50 Russian–English biliterates through a reading-aloud task with familiar and novel word forms repeated across 10 blocks. There were three Script conditions: (1) native Cyrillic, (2) non-native Roman, and (3) Ambiguous (with graphically identical, but phonologically inconsistent graphemes shared by both alphabets). Our analysis revealed the main effect of Script on both reading and orthographic learning: naming latencies during training were longer for the ambiguous stimuli, particularly for the novel ones. Nonetheless, novel word forms in the ambiguous condition approached the latencies for the familiar words along the exposures, although this effect was faster in the phonologically consistent trials. Post-training tests revealed similarly successful performance patterns for previously familiar and newly trained forms, indicating successful rapid acquisition of the latter. Furthermore, we found the highest free recall rates for the ambiguous stimuli. Overall, our results indicate that phonological inconsistency initially interferes with the efficiency of novel word encoding. Nevertheless, it does not prevent efficient attribution of orthographic representations; instead, the knowledge of two distinct alphabets supports a more efficient learning and a better memory for ambiguous stimuli via enhancing their encoding and retrieval.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Code availability

The code used in this study is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Notes

  1. German: n = 19; French: n = 11; Spanish: n = 6; Latin n = 4; Italian: n = 3; Ukrainian: n = 3; Arab: n = 2; Armenian: n = 1; Chinese: n = 1; Swedish: n = 1; Indonesian: n = 1; Czech: n = 1; Belorussian: n = 1; Danish: n = 1.

References

  • Abdelhadi, S., Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading of Arabic: Effects of orthographic visual complexity on detection. Writing Systems Research, 3(2), 117–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Álvarez-Cañizo, M., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2018). The role of sublexical variables in reading fluency development among Spanish children. Journal of Child Language, 45(4), 858–877. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000917000514.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Álvarez-Cañizo, M., Suárez-Coalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2019). Orthographic learning in Spanish children: Influence of previous semantic and phonological knowledge. Journal of Research in Reading, 42(1), 137–149. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9817.12254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ando, E., Jared, D., Nakayama, M., & Hino, Y. (2014). Cross-script phonological priming with Japanese Kanji primes and English targets. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(8), 853–870.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angwin, A. J., Phua, B., & Copland, D. A. (2014). Using semantics to enhance new word learning: An ERP investigation. Neuropsychologia, 59, 169–178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakker, I., Takashima, A., van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J. M. (2015). Tracking lexical consolidation with ERPs: Lexical and semantic-priming effects on N400 and LPC responses to newly-learned words. Neuropsychologia, 79, 33–41.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., & Baayen, H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv preprint arXiv:1506.04967.

  • Bermúdez-Margaretto, B., Beltrán, D., Shtyrov, Y., Dominguez, A., & Cuetos, F. (2020). Neurophysiological correlates of top-down phonological and semantic influence during the orthographic processing of novel visual word-forms. Brain Sciences, 10(10), 717.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the. Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing, 185, 2–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2014). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.2.01) [Computer program].

  • Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J., & Hanley, D. A. (2005). Interfering neighbours: The impact of novel word learning on the identification of visually similar words. Cognition, 97(3), B45–B54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., Van Dyck, G., & Van de Poel, M. (1999). Visual word recognition in bilinguals: Evidence from masked phonological priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 137.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bultena, S., Dijkstra, T., & van Hell, J. G. (2013). Cognate and word class ambiguity effects in noun and verb processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(9), 1350–1377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, S. C., Chen, X., & Geva, E. (2019). Deconstructing and reconstructing cross-language transfer in bilingual reading development: An interactive framework. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50, 149–161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clay, F., Bowers, J. S., Davis, C. J., & Hanley, D. A. (2007). Teaching adults new words: The role of practice and consolidation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(5), 970.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cop, U., Dirix, N., Van Assche, E., Drieghe, D., & Duyck, W. (2017). Reading a book in one or two languages? An eye movement study of cognate facilitation in L1 and L2 reading. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(4), 747–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Core, R. T., & Team, R. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

  • Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Share, D. L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82(3), 185–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00008-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & Share, D. L. (2007). Orthographic learning during oral and silent reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(1), 55–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diemand-Yauman, C., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Vaughan, E. B. (2011). Fortune favors the (): Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition, 118(1), 111–115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In Gough PB, Ehri LC, & Treiman R (Eds.), Reading Acquisition (pp. 107–143). Routledge.

  • Eskenazi, M. A., & Nix, B. (2020). Individual differences in the desirable difficulty effect during lexical acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(1), 45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FitzPatrick, I., & Indefrey, P. (2010). Lexical competition in nonnative speech comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1165–1178.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Chambers, S. M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(6), 627–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J., Weisberg, S., Friendly, M., Hong, J., Andersen, R., Firth, D., & Fox, M. J. (2016). Package ‘effects’. http://www.r-project.orghttp://socserv.socsci.mcmaster.ca/jfox. Retrieved 18 Apr 2020.

  • Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (2016). Phonological skills and learning to read. Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamada, M., & Koda, K. (2008). Influence of first language orthographic experience on second language decoding and word learning. Language Learning, 58(1), 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havas, V., Taylor, J. S. H., Vaquero, L., de Diego-Balaguer, R., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., & Davis, M. H. (2018). Semantic and phonological schema influence spoken word learning and overnight consolidation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(6), 1469–1481.

    Google Scholar 

  • Havelka, J., & Rastle, K. (2005). The assembly of phonology from print is serial and subject to strategic control: Evidence from Serbian. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(1), 148.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoenig, J. M., & Heisey, D. M. (2001). The abuse of power: the pervasive fallacy of power calculations for data analysis. The American Statistician, 55(1), 19–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44(1), 2–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., & Szucs, C. (2002). Phonological activation in bilinguals: Evidence from interlingual homograph naming. Bilingualism, 5(3), 225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability. (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jouravlev, O., Lupker, S. J., & Jared, D. (2014). Cross-language phonological activation: Evidence from masked onset priming and ERPs. Brain and Language, 134, 11–22.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahn-Horwitz, J., Kuash, S., Ibrahim, R., & Schwartz, M. (2014). How do previously acquired languages affect acquisition of English as a foreign language: The case of Circassian. Written Language & Literacy, 17(1), 40–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khare, V., Verma, A., Kar, B., Srinivasan, N., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Bilingualism and the increased attentional blink effect: Evidence that the difference between bilinguals and monolinguals generalizes to different levels of second language proficiency. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 77(6), 728–737.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J., & Davis, C. (2003). Task effects in masked cross-script translation and phonological priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 484–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kwok, R. K. W., Cuetos, F., Avdyli, R., & Ellis, A. W. (2017). Reading and lexicalization in opaque and transparent orthographies: Word naming and word learning in English and Spanish. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(10), 2105–2129. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1223705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwok, R. K. W., & Ellis, A. W. (2015). Visual word learning in skilled readers of English. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(2), 326–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.944549

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kyte, C. S., & Johnson, C. J. (2006). The role of phonological recoding in orthographic learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93(2), 166–185.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lallier, M., & Carreiras, M. (2018). Cross-linguistic transfer in bilinguals reading in two alphabetic orthographies: The grain size accommodation hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 386–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemhöfer, K., & Broersma, M. (2012). Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid lexical test for advanced learners of English. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 325–343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leminen, A., Kimppa, L., Leminen, M. M., Lehtonen, M., Mäkelä, J. P., & Shtyrov, Y. (2016). Acquisition and consolidation of novel morphology in human neocortex: A neuromagnetic study. Cortex, 83, 1–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lukatela, G. (1999). Effects of frequency and phonological ambiguity on naming Serbo-Croatian words. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maloney, E., Risko, E. F., O’Malley, S., & Besner, D. (2009). Short article: Tracking the transition from sublexical to lexical processing: On the creation of orthographic and phonological lexical representations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(5), 858–867. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802578385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50 (4), 940-967.

  • Martens, V. E. G., & De Jong, P. F. (2008). Effects of repeated reading on the length effect in word and pseudoword reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(1), 40–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Zhou, Y., Cho, J.-R., Aram, D., Levin, I., & Tolchinsky, L. (2011). Visual spatial skill: A consequence of learning to read? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 256–262.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., & Kim, A. (2004). Neural correlates of second-language word learning: Minimal instruction produces rapid change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703–704.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merkx, M., Rastle, K., & Davis, M. H. (2011). The acquisition of morphological knowledge investigated through artificial language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(6), 1200–1220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mestres-Missé, A., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. F. (2007). Watching the Brain during Meaning Acquisition. Cerebral Cortex, 17(8), 1858–1866.

  • Modirkhamene, S. (2006). The reading achievement of third language versus second language learners of English in relation to the interdependence hypothesis. International Journal of Multilingualism, 3(4), 280–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nag, S. (2007). Early reading in Kannada: The pace of acquisition of orthographic knowledge and phonemic awareness. Journal of Research in Reading, 30(1), 7–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama, M., Sears, C. R., Hino, Y., & Lupker, S. J. (2012). Cross-script phonological priming for Japanese-English bilinguals: Evidence for integrated phonological representations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(10), 1563–1583.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partanen, E. J., Leminen, A., Cook, C., & Shtyrov, Y. (2018). Formation of neocortical memory circuits for unattended written word forms: Neuromagnetic evidence. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 15829.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cotelli, M., Cossu, G., Corte, F., & Lorusso, M. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3(1), 91–96.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, D., Dijkstra, T., & Grainger, J. (2013). The representation and processing of identical cognates by late bilinguals: RT and ERP effects. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(4), 315–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(1), 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0701_02

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Dunlap, S. (2008). Learning to read: General principles and writing system variations. In Koda K & Zehler AM (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 25–50). New York, NY: Routledge.

  • Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L.-H. (2002). How the mind can meet the brain in reading: A comparative writing systems approach. In Kao HSR, Leong CK, Gao DG (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience Studies of the Chinese Language, 35–60. Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press.

  • Qiao, X., & Forster, K. I. (2013). Novel word lexicalization and the prime lexicality effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(4), 1064.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Qiao, X., Forster, K., & Witzel, N. (2009). Is banara really a word? Cognition, 113(2), 254–257.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., Havelka, J., Wydell, T. N., Coltheart, M., & Besner, D. (2009). The cross-script length effect: Further evidence challenging PDP models of reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(1), 238.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reverberi, C., Kuhlen, A. K., Seyed-Allaei, S., Greulich, R. S., Costa, A., Abutalebi, J., & Haynes, J. D. (2018). The neural basis of free language choice in bilingual speakers: Disentangling language choice and language execution. NeuroImage, 177, 108–116.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salasoo, A., Shiffrin, R. M., & Feustel, T. C. (1985). Building permanent memory codes: Codification and repetition effects in word identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114(1), 50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schad, D. J., Vasishth, S., Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R. (2020). How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) models: A tutorial. Journal of Memory and Language, 110, 104038.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime: User’s guide. Psychology Software Incorporated.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M., Geva, E., Share, D. L., & Leikin, M. (2007). Learning to read in English as third language: The cross-linguistic transfer of phonological processing skills. Written Language & Literacy, 10(1), 25–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M., Kahn-Horwitz, J., & Share, D. L. (2014). Orthographic learning and self-teaching in a bilingual and biliterate context. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117(1), 45–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.08.008

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., Erskine, J. M., & Network, C. with C. A. A. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(2), 143–174.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008a). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008b). Orthographic learning, phonological recoding, and self-teaching. In Advances in child development and behavior (vol. 36, pp. 31–82). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

  • Share, D L, & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1–57.

  • Snowling, M. J., & Göbel, S. M. (2011). Reading development and dyslexia.

  • Suárez-Coalla, P., Álvarez-Cañizo, M., & Cuetos, F. (2016). Orthographic learning in Spanish children. Journal of Research in Reading, 39(3), 292–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Takashima, A., Bakker, I., Van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., & McQueen, J. M. (2014). Richness of information about novel words influences how episodic and semantic memory networks interact during lexicalization. NeuroImage, 84, 265–278.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamura, N., Castles, A., & Nation, K. (2017). Orthographic learning, fast and slow: Lexical competition effects reveal the time course of word learning in developing readers. Cognition, 163, 93–102.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Daal, V. H. P., & Wass, M. (2017). First- and second-language learnability explained by orthographic depth and orthographic learning: A “Natural” Scandinavian experiment. Scientific Studies of Reading, 21(1), 46–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Der Meij, M., Cuetos, F., Carreiras, M., & Barber, H. A. (2011). Electrophysiological correlates of language switching in second language learners. Psychophysiology, 48(1), 44–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, S., & Nicenboim, B. (2016). Statistical methods for linguistic research: Foundational ideas—part I. Language and Linguistics Compass, 10(8), 349–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Studnitz, R. E., & Green, D. W. (2002). Interlingual homograph interference in German-English bilinguals: Its modulation and locus of control. Bilingualism, 5(1), 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagenmakers, E. J. (2007). Stopping rules and their irrelevance for Bayesian inference: Online appendix to “A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values.”. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 1–5.

  • Wang, H.-C., Castles, A., & Nickels, L. (2012). Word regularity affects orthographic learning. SAGE Publications Sage UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wen, Y., & van Heuven, W. J. (2017). Non-cognate translation priming in masked priming lexical decision experiments: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(3), 879–886.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, H., Chen, B., Yang, M., & Dunlap, S. (2010). Language nonselective access to phonological representations: Evidence from Chinese-English bilinguals. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(10), 2051–2066.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The study was funded by a grant from the Russian Science Foundation (project no. 19-78-00140) awarded to HSE University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Beatriz Bermúdez-Margaretto.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethics approval

The manuscript does not contain clinical studies or patient data. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Psychology, National Research University Higher School of Economics.

Consent to participate

All participants gave their written consent to take part in the study.

Consent for publication

All authors have approved the manuscript and agree with its publication.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 189 kb)

Appendix: Stimuli used in the study. Note that the same handwritten font was used across all tasks

Appendix: Stimuli used in the study. Note that the same handwritten font was used across all tasks

figure a

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bermúdez-Margaretto, B., Kopytin, G., Myachykov, A. et al. Biliteracy and acquisition of novel written words: the impact of phonological conflict between L1 and L2 scripts. Psychological Research 86, 871–890 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01529-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01529-y

Navigation